A leading scholar, Evelyn Fox Keller is professor emerita of history and philosophy of science, Massachusetts Institute of Technology in the Program in Science, Technology and Society. Her research focuses on the history and philosophy of modern biology and on gender and science, while her newest book, The Mirage of a Space Between Nature and Nurture, makes a profound critique of the debates over the influence of heredity and environment in human development and challenges our scientific understanding of the issues.
This spring, Keller is at Smith as the 2012 William Allan Neilson Chair of Research and delivers her final lecture titled “Self-Organization and God” at 4:30 p.m. April 10 in Neilson Browsing Room. She recently responded to questions from Insight about her research, including her reflections on the “puzzling persistence of the ‘nature-nurture debate.’”
Evelyn Fox Keller
Q. What do you want people to understand the most about the nature-nurture debate?
A. That nature and nurture are not separable causal streams—that it does not make sense to
ask how much of x is due to nature and how much to nurture.
Q. What should we be asking then? What are we NOT considering that we should be considering?
In my view, the most important thing we have learned over the last decades is that living
systems work through an enormously intricate dynamic of molecular interactions, and that
it is this interactivity that gives rise to, even defines, biological function. Even if
one wants to say that life begins with DNA, one still could not say that life begins with
genes. Genes are entities that result from the biological organization of the living cell. However
productive a linear view of causality may have been in the early days, such a view is no
longer adequate for the kinds of complexity biologists have uncovered.
Q. In your writings, you remind us about the power and importance of language in the debates.
Is it the language that has caused the most confusion and disagreement in the scientific
explanations?
A. Yes, the slippage between different meanings of the terms and the questions is in the
scientific discourse as well as in the public discourse.
Q. Can you give us an example of that? For instance, is it in the way we discuss medical
genetics and what we mean by the term “gene”?
Well, yes. For example, although we have known for some time now that only a small part
of the genetic material of humans (1.2%) is devoted to protein coding sequences, and that
the bulk of the genome is concerned with regulation, we continue to focus most of our attention
on protein coding sequences. This is especially true in medical genetics and in evolutionary
studies. Most diseases, as well as most evolutionary advances, however, result from changes
elsewhere in the genome.
Q. Do you believe the study of contemporary genetics can provide a useful vocabulary for
resolving some of those issues in the nature-nurture debate?
A. Yes, I do think the contemporary research is forcing us to forge a new vocabulary that
will help avoid many of the pitfalls we have been stuck in. But we are not there yet.
Q. What is the difference between the way scientists talk about nature and nurture and the
way it was discussed when you were an undergraduate?
A. There is more lip service paid these days to the idea that nature and nurture are always
in interaction, but I don't think that goes far enough—it still preserves the belief
that there are separate entities, nature and nurture, that can interact. I don't think it
is possible to meaningfully define either term in the absence of the other.
Q. What is your recommendation to the aspiring scientist as well as to those who simply
want to more clearly understand the natural world?
A. To think hard about the questions they are trying to address, and about the assumptions
built into the ways in which the questions are posed. One of the most interesting questions
in biology, as well as one of the questions that most preoccupies lay citizens interested
in questions about nature and nurture, is how plastic an organism is. How malleable is a
baby? How sensitive (or responsive) is he or she likely to be to changes in her environment?
We have learned that very little about us is in fact fixed at birth, but some things may
require enormous effort to change. Most behavioral traits however are quite malleable—the
trick is to learn what they are responsive to.